|
|
(One intermediate revision by one other user not shown) |
Line 1: |
Line 1: |
| In [[visual perception|ocular]] [[physiology]], '''adaptation''' is the ability of the [[human eye|eye]] to adjust to various levels of darkness and light.
| | Adrianne Swoboda is what your ex husband loves to e-mail her though she often really like being [http://data.gov.uk/data/search?q=medically medically] known as like that. After being out of their job for years he became an order worker. What your loves doing is to go to karaoke but she is thinking on starting something new. Massachusetts is often where he's always lived. She is running and reduce a blog here: http://prometeu.net<br><br> |
|
| |
|
| == Efficiency ==
| | My webpage - [http://prometeu.net clash of clans hack cydia] |
| The human eye can function from very dark to very bright levels of light; its sensing capabilities reach across nine [[order of magnitude|orders of magnitude]]. This means that the brightest and the darkest light signal that the eye can sense are a factor of roughly 1,000,000,000 apart. However, in any given moment of time, the eye can only sense a [[contrast (vision)|contrast]] ratio of one thousand. What enables the wider reach is that the eye adapts its definition of what is black.
| |
| | |
| The eye takes approximately 20–30 minutes to fully adapt from bright sunlight to complete darkness and become ten thousand to one million times more sensitive than at full daylight. In this process, the eye's perception of color changes as well (this is called the [[Purkinje effect]]). However, it takes approximately five minutes for the eye to adapt to bright sunlight from darkness. This is due to cones obtaining more sensitivity when first entering the dark for the first five minutes but the rods take over after five or more minutes.<ref name="eye, human">"Sensory Reception: Human Vision: Structure and Function of the Human Eye" Encyclopaedia Brtinnica, vol. 27, 1987</ref>
| |
| | |
| == Ambient Light Response ==
| |
| [[File:Dark Adaptation.png|thumbnail|right|Visual Response to Darkness]]
| |
| A minor mechanism of adaptation is the [[pupillary light reflex]], adjusting the amount of light that reaches the retina.
| |
| | |
| In response to varying ambient light levels, [[rods and cones]] of eye function both in isolation and in tandem to adjust the visual system. Changes in the sensitivity of [[rods and cones]] in the eye are the major contributors to dark adaptation.
| |
| | |
| Above a certain [[luminance]] level (about 0.03 cd/m2), the cone mechanism is involved in mediating vision; [[photopic vision]]. Below this level, the rod mechanism comes into play providing [[scotopic vision|scotopic (night) vision]]. The range where two mechanisms are working together is called the [[Mesopic vision|mesopic range]], as there is not an abrupt transition between the two mechanism. This adaptation forms the basis of the '''Duplicity Theory'''<ref name="Light_Dark">http://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-viii-gabac-receptors/light-and-dark-adaptation/"Light and Dark Adaptation"</ref>
| |
| | |
| == Dark Adaptation ==
| |
| [[Rhodopsin]], a biological pigment in the photoreceptors of the retina immediately [[Photobleaching|photobleaches]] in response to light.<ref name="Stuart_1996">{{cite book | author = Stuart JA, Brige RR | authorlink = | editor = Lee AG | others = | title = Rhodopsin and G-Protein Linked Receptors, Part A (Vol 2, 1996) (2 Vol Set) | edition = | language = | publisher = JAI Press | location = Greenwich, Conn | year = 1996 | origyear = | pages = 33–140 | quote = | isbn = 1-55938-659-2 | chapter = Characterization of the primary photochemical events in bacteriorhodopsin and rhodopsin | doi = | url = | accessdate = }}</ref> [[Rod cells|Rods]] are more sensitive to light and so take longer to fully adapt to the change in light. [[Rod cells|Rods]], whose photopigments regenerate more slowly, do not reach their maximum sensitivity for about half an hour.<ref name="eye, human"/><ref>{{cite book|last=Passer and Smith|title=Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior|year=2008|edition=4th|page=135|isbn=0-07-256334-6}}</ref> [[Cone cells|Cones]] take approximately 9–10 minutes to adapt to the dark.<ref name="eye, human"/>
| |
| Sensitivity to light is modulated by changes in intracellular [[calcium]] [[ion]]s and [[cyclic guanosine monophosphate]].<ref name="Hurley">{{cite journal | last=Hurley | first=JB | title=Shedding Light on Adaptation | journal=Journal of General Physiology | volume=119 | issue=2 | pages=125–128 |date=February 2002 | pmid=11815663 | doi=10.1085/jgp.119.2.125 | pmc=2233798 }}</ref>
| |
| | |
| The sensitivity of the [[Rod cells|rod pathway]] improves considerably within 5–10 minutes in the dark. Color testing has been used to determine the time at which [[Rod cells|rod]] mechanism takes over, when the rod mechanism takes over colored spots appear colorless as only [[Cone cell|cone pathways]] encode color.<ref>Aubert H. Physiologie der Netzhaut. Breslau: E. Morgenstern; 1865.</ref>
| |
| | |
| Four factors affect dark adaptation:
| |
| | |
| :'''Intensity and Duration of the pre-adapting light'''
| |
| By increasing the levels of pre-adapting luminances, the duration of cone mechanism dominance extends, while the rod mechanism switch over is more delayed. In addition the absolute threshold takes longer to reach. The opposite is true for decreasing the levels of pre-adapting luminances.<ref name="saturation">Bartlett NR. Dark and light adaptation. In: Graham CH, editor. Vision and visual perception. New
| |
| York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.; 1965.</ref>
| |
| | |
| :'''Size and Location of the Retina'''
| |
| The location of the test spot affects the dark adaptation curve because of the distribution of the [[Photoreceptor cell|rods and cones]] in the retina.<ref>Hallett PE. The variations in visual threshold measurement. J Physiol 1969;202:403–419.1351489 [PubMed: 5784294]</ref>
| |
| | |
| :'''Wavelength of the Threshold Light'''
| |
| Varying the wavelengths of stimuli also effect the dark adaptation curve. Long wavelengths, such as extreme red, create the absence of a distinct rod/cone break as the [[Photoreceptor cell|rod and cones cells]] have similar sensitives to light of long wavelengths. Conversely at short wavelengths the rod/cone break is more prominent because the [[rod cells]] are much more sensitive than cones once the [[rod cells|rods]] have dark adapted.<ref name="saturation"/>
| |
| | |
| :'''Rhodopsin Regeneration'''
| |
| Dark adaptation depends upon [[photopigment]] bleaching, which effects the threshold of both [[Photoreceptor cell|cones and rods]].<ref name="saturation"/>
| |
| | |
| ===Inhibition===
| |
| Inhibition by neurons also affects activation in synapses. Together with the bleaching of a [[Photoreceptor cell|rod or cone pigment]], merging of signals on [[ganglion cells]] are inhibited, reducing convergence. Alpha adaptation, i.e. rapid sensitivity fluctuations, is powered by nerve control. The merging of signals by virtue of the diffuse [[ganglion cells]], as well as horizontal and amacrine cells, allow a cumulative effect. Thus that area of stimulation is inversely proportional to intensity of light, a strong stimulus of 100 rods equivalent to a weak stimulus of 1,000 rods.
| |
| | |
| In sufficiently bright light, convergence is low, but during dark adaptation, convergence of rod signals boost. This is not due to structural changes, but by a possible shutdown of inhibition that stops convergence of messages in bright light. If only one eye is open, the closed eye must adapt separately upon reopening to match the already adapted eye.<ref name="eye, human"/>
| |
| | |
| ==Light Adaptation==
| |
| [[File:Light adaptation.png|thumbnail|left|Schematic of the increment threshold curve of the rod system]]
| |
| With light adaptation, the eye has to quickly adapt to the background illumination to be able to distinguish objects in this background. The process for light adaptation occurs over a period of five minutes.
| |
| | |
| '''Photochemical Reaction:'''
| |
| ::<math>\mathrm{Rhodopsin \rightleftharpoons \ Retinal + Opsin}</math>
| |
| | |
| === Increment Threshold ===
| |
| Using increment threshold experiments, light adaptation can be measured clinically.<ref>H Davson. Physiology of the eye. 5th ed. London: Macmillan Academic and Professional Ltd.; 1990.</ref> In an increment threshold experiment, a test stimulus is presented on a background of a certain [[luminance]], the stimulus is increased until the detection threshold is reached against the background. A monophasic or biphasic threshold versus intensity TVI curve is obtained through this method for both [[Photoreceptor cell|cones and rods]].
| |
| | |
| When the threshold curve for a single system, [[i.e.]] [[Photoreceptor cell|just cones or just rods]], is taken in isolation it can been seen to possesses four sections:<ref>Aguilar M, Stiles WS. Saturation of the rod mechanism of the reina at high levels of stimulation. Opt Acta (Lond) 1954;1:59–65.</ref>
| |
| | |
| :'''1. Dark Light'''
| |
| The threshold in this portion of the TVI curve is determined by the dark/light level. Sensitivity is limited by neural noise. The background field is relatively low and does not significantly affect threshold.
| |
| | |
| :'''2. Square Root Law'''
| |
| This part of the curve is limited by quantal fluctuation in the background. The visual system is usually compared with a theoretical construct called the ideal light detector. To detect the stimulus, the stimulus must sufficiently exceed the fluctuations of the background (noise).
| |
| | |
| :'''3. Weber's Law'''
| |
| Threshold increases with background [[luminance]] proportional to the square root of the background.<ref>Barlow HB. Increment thresholds at low intensities considered as signal noise discriminations. J Physiol 1958;141:337–350. [PubMed: 13539843]</ref>
| |
| | |
| :'''4. Saturation'''
| |
| At saturation, the rod system becomes unable to detect the stimulus. This section of the curve occurs for the cone mechanism under high background levels.<ref>H Davson. Physiology of the eye. 5th ed. London: Macmillan Academic and Professional Ltd.; 1990</ref>
| |
| | |
| ==Insufficiency==
| |
| Insufficiency of adaptation most commonly presents as insufficient adaptation to dark environment, called [[night blindness]] or ''nyctalopia''. The opposite problem, known as [[hemeralopia]], that is, inability to see clearly in bright light, is much rarer.
| |
| | |
| The [[Fovea centralis in macula|fovea]] is blind to dim light (due to its cone-only array) and the rods are more sensitive, so a dim star on a moonless night must be [[Averted vision|viewed from the side]], so it stimulates the rods. This is not due to pupil width since an artificial fixed-width pupil gives the same results.<ref name="eye, human"/>
| |
| | |
| ==See also==
| |
| *[[Neural adaptation]]
| |
| *[[Purkinje effect]]
| |
| *[[Photopic vision]]
| |
| *[[Mesopic vision]]
| |
| *[[Scotopic vision]]
| |
| *[[Accommodation (eye)]]
| |
| | |
| ==References==
| |
| {{reflist}}
| |
| | |
| ==External links==
| |
| *{{MeshName|Adaptation,+Ocular}}
| |
| *http://webvision.med.utah.edu/light_dark.html
| |
| | |
| [[Category:Animal physiology]]
| |
| [[Category:Eye]]
| |
Adrianne Swoboda is what your ex husband loves to e-mail her though she often really like being medically known as like that. After being out of their job for years he became an order worker. What your loves doing is to go to karaoke but she is thinking on starting something new. Massachusetts is often where he's always lived. She is running and reduce a blog here: http://prometeu.net
My webpage - clash of clans hack cydia